Although
the mutational study of behavioral characters was initiated by Seymour Benzer
in the 1960’s, his study was concerned primarily with laboratory mutations of fruitflies
and did not give much insight into the evolutionary mechanisms of behavioral
characters in the wild. For this reason, many authors are now investigating
this problem at the molecular level. Horton et al. (2014) recently published an
interesting result with respect to the social behavior in the white-throated
sparrow Zonotrichia albicollis.
In the
white-throated sparrow there are two polymorphic phenotypes with respect to the
color pattern of the head crown: (1) tan-striped (TS) and (2) white-striped (WS)
(Fig. 1). The male WS phenotype is
known to be more aggressive than the male TS
with respect to territoriality and mate-finding. In song-birds, these behaviors
are typically dependent on sex steroid during the breeding season. WS birds have higher plasma testosterone
than the same sex TS birds. However, morph
differences in behavior cannot be entirely explained by these hormones, because
the differences persist even when plasma levels are experimentally equalized. Therefore,
individual variation in steroid-dependent behavior may be better explained by
neural sensitivity to the hormones, for example by variation in the
distribution and abundance of steroid receptors (Horton et al., 2014).
From Horton et al. (2014). |
By the way, the WS and TS are associated with two inversion haplotypes of chromosome 2
(Thomas et al. 2008). That is, the genes controlling WS and TS are apparently located
in the inverted segment of haplotypes ZAL2m
and ZAL2. However, because WS is dominant over TS and the frequency of ZAL2m
is relatively low, individuals can roughly be divided into two groups, WS (ZAL2m/ZAL2,) and TS (ZAL2/ZAL2), genotype ZAL2m/ZAL2m,
being practically absent. It has also been inferred that ZAL2m was derived from ZAL2 about 2 million years ago by chromosomal inversion and therefore
the polymorphism has existed for a long time. Note also that there is practically
no recombination between two inverted chromosomes.
Horton et al.
(2014) looked for steroid receptor genes in the inverted segment and found that
the gene (ESR1) encoding estrogen
receptor α (ERα) is located in the
inverted segment and that the two proteins encoded by the ESR1 genes from the WS
and TS phenotypes showed one amino
acid difference but this difference did not affect the gene expression pattern appreciably.
They then hypothesized that the phenotypic difference between WS and TS is caused by the difference in the gene regulatory region of the
gene. In fact, when the binding sites of transcription factors in the cis-regulatory region upstream of the ESR1 gene were examined by a computer
program, there was considerable difference between the WS and TS haplotypes
(Fig. 2A).
From Horton et al. (2014). |
However, to prove
that this difference is indeed responsible for the behavioral difference, it
was necessary to show that the expression level of the ESR1 gene is higher in haplotype ZAL2m than in ZAL2.
For this purpose, Horton et al. used several molecular techniques such as the
luciferase reporter method with HeLa cells and radioimmunoassay. Their results showed
that the expression level of the ESR1
gene is about 1.5 times higher in haplotype ZAL2m
than in ZAL2 (Fig. 2B,C).
This finding
indicates that the difference in expression level of a single major gene
generates a clear phenotypic difference, which in turn affects an important
behavioral character. At the present time, this type of data is rare, but it is
possible that many behavioral characters are controlled by similar molecular
mechanisms, and it is desirable that more studies will be conducted in the
future. In practice, behavioral characters are generally controlled by many
genes, and eventually we may be able to understand the molecular basis of the characters.
Yet, the basic principle of gene expression could be simpler than our intuition
suggests as in the case of the above example. In their “significance”
statement, Horton et al. write:
In this series of
studies, we provide a rare illustration of how a chromosomal polymorphism has
affected overt social behavior in a vertebrate. White-throated sparrows occur
in two alternative phenotypes, or morphs, distinguished by a chromosomal
rearrangement. That the morphs differ in territorial and parental behavior has
been known for decades, but how the rearrangement affects behavior is not understood.
Here we show that genetic differentiation between the morphs affects the
transcription of a gene well known to be involved in social behavior. We then
show that in a free-living population, the neural expression of this gene
predicts both territorial and parental behavior. We hypothesize that this
mechanism has played a causal role in the evolution of alternative life-history
strategies. |
No comments:
Post a Comment